Page 12 |
Save page Remove page | Previous | 12 of 34 | Next |
|
small (250x250 max)
medium (500x500 max)
Large (1000x1000 max)
Extra Large
large ( > 500x500)
Full Resolution
All (PDF)
|
This page
All
|
5 Many people will be familiar with some of embodied cognition‘s effects. The experience of feeling transported to a time and place in the distant past as a result of hearing a song, smelling an odor, or tasting a flavor one associates with the past, is a consequence of embodied cognition. Other effects become clear through research – studies that reveal how entwined perception, action and introspective states are such that the triggering of one prompts the expression and experience of another. For example, several researchers have shown that priming the elderly stereotype prompts people to walk and to complete lexical decision tasks more slowly than those in whom the stereotype had not been primed.2 Study participants asked to recall an unethical deed were more likely to take antiseptic wipes and complete word tasks with cleansing-related words than those who recalled an ethical action.3 Participants in a group of studies looking at the effects of holding heavier versus lighter clipboards showed that those who held a heavier clipboard: (1) judged higher the value of foreign currencies, (2) judged more important the employment of fair decision-making practices, (3) gave more consistent judgments on test questions (revealing that they had given them deeper thought), (4) were less tolerant of weak (compared to strong) arguments, and (5) judged prospective job candidates as better overall and more serious about performing the job in quality – than those participants who 2 JA Bargh, M Chen, and L Burrows, ‘Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action’ in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 71 (1996) pp. 230-244. cited in Niedenthal, Barsalou 2005, p. 184 3 CB Zhong and K Liljenquist ‘Washing Away Your Sins: Threatened Morality and Physical Cleansing’ in Science Vol. 313, (8 September 2006) pp. 1451-1452.
Object Description
Title | Toward a theory of gesture design |
Author | Tucker, Diane |
Author email | diane.tucker@gmail.com; dmtucker@usc.edu |
Degree | Master of Fine Arts |
Document type | Thesis |
Degree program | Interactive Media |
School | School of Cinematic Arts |
Date defended/completed | 2011-05-02 |
Date submitted | 2011 |
Restricted until | Unrestricted |
Date published | 2011-05-04 |
Advisor (committee chair) | Bolas, Mark |
Advisor (committee member) |
Fullerton, Tracy Kratky, Andreas Malamed, Laird |
Abstract | The enormous transformation in how humans engage with technologies – providing direct access through touch or gesture, without any mediating controller – has just reached mainstream computing, games and home theaters, with the recent releases of the Kinect and the WAVI Xtion. This change has opened up huge new opportunities for the design of games, interactive experiences and applications. This paper presents the evidence of the connection between the body and perceptions, emotions, and mental states; the powerful, extensive, and surprising ways those connections are manifest; and the unexpected and very potent role that metaphor plays. This paper then presents how that evidence points to a way of employing the emotional and cognitive armature attached to human movement as a means of developing emotionally compelling gestural game-play. |
Keyword | game design; gesture; gestural vocabulary; gestural design; gesture design; user interface design; human computer interaction; human centered computing; emotion in games; design; metaphor |
Coverage date | 1990/2011 |
Language | English |
Part of collection | University of Southern California dissertations and theses |
Publisher (of the original version) | University of Southern California |
Place of publication (of the original version) | Los Angeles, California |
Publisher (of the digital version) | University of Southern California. Libraries |
Provenance | Electronically uploaded by the author |
Type | texts |
Legacy record ID | usctheses-m3891 |
Contributing entity | University of Southern California |
Rights | Tucker, Diane |
Repository name | Libraries, University of Southern California |
Repository address | Los Angeles, California |
Repository email | cisadmin@lib.usc.edu |
Filename | etd-tucker-4587 |
Archival file | uscthesesreloadpub_Volume29/etd-tucker-4587.pdf |
Description
Title | Page 12 |
Contributing entity | University of Southern California |
Repository email | cisadmin@lib.usc.edu |
Full text | 5 Many people will be familiar with some of embodied cognition‘s effects. The experience of feeling transported to a time and place in the distant past as a result of hearing a song, smelling an odor, or tasting a flavor one associates with the past, is a consequence of embodied cognition. Other effects become clear through research – studies that reveal how entwined perception, action and introspective states are such that the triggering of one prompts the expression and experience of another. For example, several researchers have shown that priming the elderly stereotype prompts people to walk and to complete lexical decision tasks more slowly than those in whom the stereotype had not been primed.2 Study participants asked to recall an unethical deed were more likely to take antiseptic wipes and complete word tasks with cleansing-related words than those who recalled an ethical action.3 Participants in a group of studies looking at the effects of holding heavier versus lighter clipboards showed that those who held a heavier clipboard: (1) judged higher the value of foreign currencies, (2) judged more important the employment of fair decision-making practices, (3) gave more consistent judgments on test questions (revealing that they had given them deeper thought), (4) were less tolerant of weak (compared to strong) arguments, and (5) judged prospective job candidates as better overall and more serious about performing the job in quality – than those participants who 2 JA Bargh, M Chen, and L Burrows, ‘Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action’ in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 71 (1996) pp. 230-244. cited in Niedenthal, Barsalou 2005, p. 184 3 CB Zhong and K Liljenquist ‘Washing Away Your Sins: Threatened Morality and Physical Cleansing’ in Science Vol. 313, (8 September 2006) pp. 1451-1452. |